Cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness associations with older adolescent cognitive controlT

2021-02-03 07:08atsuyaShigetaAngusLeahyJordanSmithNarelleEatherDavidLuansCharlesHillmancT
Journal of Sport and Health Science 2021年1期

atsuya T.Shigeta*,Angus A.Leahy,Jordan J.Smith,Narelle Eather,David R.Luans,Charles H.HillmancT

aDepartment of Psychology,Northeastern University,Boston,MA 02115,USA

bPriority Research Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition,School of Education,University of Newcastle,Callaghan,NSW 2308,Australia

cDepartment of Physical Therapy,Movement&Rehabilitation Sciences,Northeastern University,Boston,MA 02131,USA T

Received 13 December 2019;revised 17 March 2020;accepted 24 April 2020 Available online 20 May 2020

2095-2546/©2021 Published by Elsevier B.V.on behalf of Shanghai University of Sport.This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license.(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)

Abstract Background:Participation in physical activity supports greater cardiorespiratory fitness(CRF),a correlate of cognitive control.However,the relationship between muscular fitness(MF)and cognitive control is less clear.The present study investigated the differential relationship of CRF and MF with cognitive control in older adolescents.Methods:This cross-sectional study involved students(15-17 years old,n=541,43%female)from 20 secondary schools who completed tests of inhibition(modi fied flanker task),working memory(n-back task),CRF(Progressive Aerobic Cardiovascular Endurance Run),and MF(standing long jump and push-up test).Multilevel analyses tested the association between CRF or MF and cognitive outcomes while accounting for the in fluence of the other fitness variable and relevant demographic factors.Results:CRF predicted response accuracy during incongruent flanker trials,the condition requiring greater inhibition.For the working memory task,CRF predicted greater target accuracy and greater d’scores on the 1-back task,requiring lesser amounts of working memory.In the 2-back task,which requires greater amounts of working memory,CRF also predicted greater target and non-target accuracy and d’scores.Comparatively,MF did not predict any cognitive outcomes after adjustment for CRF.Conclusion:CRF was selectively related to better performance during task conditions that require greater amounts of inhibition and working memory.This finding suggests that CRF,but not MF,may bene fit cognitive control in older adolescents.This selective in fluence of CRF on older adolescents’cognition highlights the value of aerobic physical activity.

Keywords:Adolescence;Cognition;Physical activity;Physical fitness

1.Introduction

The growing trend of physical inactivity is now considered a pandemic1and is forecasted to rise even further in the coming decades.2In 2013,scholars estimated that physical inactivity accounted for at least USD68.5 billion of the global economic burden due to expenses associated with healthcare costs and productivity loss.3In adolescents,4much of these costs are a consequence of increased prevalenceofcomorbid diseases,such as type 2diabetes and the metabolic syndrome.Critically,adverse effects of physical inactivity further extend to cognitive health.5,6Participation in physical activity,in part,may prevent the onset and severity of non-communicable diseases7,8and can lead to gains in physical fitness,9including both cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness(MF).Cardiorespiratory fitness(CRF)provides a number of health benef i ts in childhood and adolescence10and predicts health status into adulthood.11Concurrently,MF(i.e.,muscle strength,endurance,and power)can bene fit indices of health in both children and adolescents.10,12The bene fits of physical fitness have also been extended to cognitive and brain health.In particular,CRF promotes greater cognitive functioning across the lifespan13and is relatedtoenhancedacademicachievement14-16andbraindevelopment in children and adolescents.17,18

Childhood CRF has been associated with cognitive control,19,20which is an umbrella term(also known as executive function,central executive,or executive control)that denotes a network of processes underlying adaptive,goal-directed behavior and is commonly categorized along the dimensions of inhibition,working memory,and mental flexibility.21Children with higher CRF often outperform less fit children on tasks requiring greater amounts of cognitive control.22While growing evidence has supported these bene fits,less is known about whether the bene fits are generalized across aspects of physical fitness or are speci fic to particular domains of fitness.Interestingly,research in children and adolescents has shown that greater CRF,but not MF,relates to higher academic achievement.15,23However,Kao and colleagues24found that greater MF,independent of CRF,predicted increased response accuracy on a working memory task,as well as on math performance,providing evidence that MF may be related to aspects of cognitive control and scholastic performance.Together,the link between CRF and cognitive control is wellestablished in the literature;however,the relationship between MF and cognitive control remains less clear.

Curiously,the literature concerning fitness and cognitive control in youth has involved preadolescent(~8-10 years old)and younger adolescent(~11-14 years old)populations,while late adolescent(~15-18 years old)populations have largely been overlooked(see references25,26for exceptions).The brain undergoes a multitude of changes from childhood to adolescence27,28and cognitive control abilities consequently develop in their complexity as well.29Thus,adolescence is vital to the development of brain mechanisms underlying cognitive control;however,it is also a volatile period of vulnerability to risk factors,including physical inactivity.Research indicates that<20%of adolescents are suf ficiently active,30which is concerning given that physical inactivity has been related to poor physical and mental health.31Moreover,poor CRF and MF are linked to cardiovascular disease prevalence and metabolic risk in adolescents.32Late adolescence also includes a period of schooling dominated by academic(e.g.,standardized testing,rigorous curriculum,etc.)and social stressors,which can hinder physical activity opportunities in schools31.In Australia,for example,the outcome is re flected in the dismal 6%of older adolescents who meet daily physical activity recommendations.33These issues warrant inquiry into late adolescents’physical fitness in relation to cognitive control.Such an analysis may provide insight into factors that protect against harmful lifestyle factors.

The current study investigated the relationship between older adolescents’physical fitness and cognitive control.Based on previous findings in studies of preadolescents,20,24,34we hypothesized that CRF and MF would both be positively associated with cognitive outcomes and that the relationships would be greater for task conditions requiring greater amounts of cognitive control.

2.Methods

2.1.Participants

Participants were recruited for a large school-based project(Burn 2 Learn)conducted in New South Wales,Australia,between March and April in both 2018 and 2019.35Ethics approval for the study was obtained from the University of Newcastle,Australia(H85 2016-0424),and from the New South Wales Department of Education(SERAP 2017116).School principals,teachers,parents,and study participants provided informed written consent prior to enrolment.The Burn 2 Learn trial was registered with the Australian and New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry(ACTRN12618000293268).Governmentschoolswith seniorschoolstudentswere recruited in 2 cohorts.In Cohort 1,ten schools located within a 90-min drive from the University of Newcastle were recruited.In Cohort 2,ten schools located within a 150-min drive from the University of Newcastle were recruited.The selected geographical regions(i.e.,Hunter-Central Coast,Sydney,Northern Sydney,Western Sydney,and New England)are broadly representative of urban and regional secondary schools in New South Wales.Eligible participants were older adolescents(15-17 years old)in Grade 11 at consenting study schools.Participants had no health or medical conditions that would preclude their participation in vigorous physical activity.Prior to testing,all participants and their parents/guardians provided written informed assent and consent,respectively.The current investigation used baseline data that was collected prior to randomization to treatment conditions.A total of 669 students were recruited and performed baseline testing.Participants who provided complete data for all measures were included in the analyses(n=541).Participants missing body mass index(n=3)and socioeconomic status(n=2)were imputed with the average within age,sex,and school.

Table 1 Demographic information on participants.

2.2.Demographics

Participants completed a questionnaire in order to provide demographic information(Table 1),including age,sex,and cultural background.Height(cm)and weight(kg)were collected to calculate body mass index(kg/m2).Socioeconomic status was determined by population tertile using the Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas,Index of Relative Socioeconomic Disadvantage on the basis of residential postcode.36For the analyses,sex(0=female,1=male)and school(1-20)were dummy coded into variables.

2.3.Cardiorespiratory assessment

The cardiorespiratory assessment was conducted by trained research assistants at the study schools during Term 1 of the school year(February-April).CRF was assessed via the 20-m Progressive Aerobic Cardiovascular Endurance Run(PACER)test using FitnessGram testing procedures(the Cooper Institute,Dallas,TX,USA).37The PACER test is the most widely used f i eld-based measure of CRF,demonstrating high reliability and validity in adolescent populations.38Participants were instructed to run back and forth between 2 sets of lines 20 m apart in accordance with an accompanying audio file.Participants were familiarized with the testing parameters,and the test began at a speed of 8.5 km/h and progressively increased by 0.5 km/h with each minute.The test continued until participants failed to maintain the required pace for 2 consecutive laps,or upon volitional fatigue.Test administrators provided verbal encouragement to participants while completing the test in order to maximize motivation.The total number of PACER laps successfully completed was used as the measure of CRF.

2.4.MF assessment

MF was assessed using measures of upper body muscular endurance and lower body muscular power.Upper body muscular endurance was assessed using a modi fied version of the push-up test(push-upscompleted),39whichhasacceptablereliabilityinadolescents.40Participants were instructed to perform as many pushups as they could in accordance with a metronome set to a cadence of 40 beats per minute.The test concluded when the participants either failed to lower themselves to the required depth on 2 nonconsecutive repetitions(warnings verbalized by assessor),failed to maintain movement coordinating with the metronome,or voluntarily ceased the exercise.The standing long jump test was used as a measure of lower body muscular power(maximum distance jumped),which has acceptable reliability and validity in adolescents.41From a standing position behind the starting line marked at 0 cm,participants were instructed to take off and land with 2 feet.Upon landing,the distance was recorded from the heel of the rearmostfoot.Participantsperformedatotalof2jumps,withthelonger of the 2 jumps providing a measure of lower body MF.42Absolute scores of push-ups and standing long jump were standardized(i.e.,(value-mean)/SD),accounting for body mass according to the weight-bearing nature of each test.43-45A measure of total MF was calculated by adding each standardized test score together.46

2.5.Cognitive tasks

Cognitive tasks were completed on Dell Latitude E5470 laptops(Dell Computers,Round Rock,TX,USA)in the classroom.All cognitive tasks were completed prior to physical fitness tests.Participants were monitored by an experimenter to ensure their understanding of the task.All tasks used the buttons“Q”and“P”.The order of tasks was counterbalanced across participants.Cognitive tasks were presented focally on a black background of a 14-inch computer screen using PsychoPy software(Version 1.83.04;PsychoPy,Nottingham,UK).47Behavioral performance data(response accuracy and reaction time(RT))for the flanker task and the 1-back and 2-back tasks were processed and reduced using MATLAB 2017a(The Math Works Inc.,Natick,MA,USA).

2.5.1 Flanker task

A modi fied flanker task48,49was used to test inhibitory control.Participants responded to the directionality of a centrally presented target arrow amid either congruent(pointing in the same direction)or incongruent(pointing in the opposite direction)flanking arrows.Participants were instructed to respond by pressing the“Q”key with their left index finger if the middle arrow faced left or the“P”key with their right index finger if the middle arrow faced right.Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible.Congruency and directionality of the stimuli were equiprobable for each block of trials.Each participant completed 25 practice trials and an additional block if a 70%response accuracy or greater was not achieved.Subsequently,1 block of 150 randomized trials were completed,where stimuli were presented for 100 ms with a variable inter-stimulus interval(900 ms,1050 ms,and 1200 ms).Stimuli consisted of five 3-cm tall white arrows.Participants were presented with one of 2 counterbalanced sets of stimuli.

2.5.2.N-back task

A serialn-back task with 1-back and 2-back conditions was used to assess working memory.In the 1-back condition,participants were instructed to view a series of shapes.If the current shape matched the previous shape(i.e.,target trial),participants responded with a “P”key press.If the current shape did not match the previous shape(i.e.,non-target trial),they responded with a“Q”key press.In the 2-back condition,participants responded to whether the stimulus from 2 trials prior(i.e.,n-2)matched the current stimulus.In each condition,a match of the 1-back or 2-back stimulus was denoted as a target trial and a non-match was denoted as a non-target trial.Each condition started with a practice block consisting of 20 trials,and an additional practice block was completed if the response accuracy was≥70%.Participants then completed 2 blocks of 72 trials(24 target,48 non-target)each for the 1-back and 2-back conditions.Stimuli were each shown for 250 ms followed by an inter-stimulus interval of 2500 ms and consisted of 3 cm colored shapes(i.e.,moon,circle,triangle,and star)on a black background.The order of the 1-back and 2-back tasks were counterbalanced across participants.

2.6.Statistical analyses

RT and response accuracy means were calculated for all cognitive measures.For the flanker task,interference effects were calculated as an index of differences in inhibitory control requirements between congruency conditions:congruent-incongruent response accuracy(i.e.,response accuracy interference)and incongruent-congruent RT(i.e.,RT interference).Thedprime(d’)was calculated as an additional measure of accuracy for the 1-back and 2-back tasks.The use ofd’is advantageous because it factors in response accuracy along with false alarm rate(i.e.,when a non-target is mistakenly given a target response).Speci fically,a formula was used to subtract thez-score of false alarm rate from thez-score of target response accuracy.50Maximum and minimum probability adjustments were made to correct for cases of 100%target accuracy(2-(1/n);n=number of trials)as well as cases with a false alarm rate of 0%(1-(2-(1/n))).A highd’score demonstrates increased discriminatory ability between target and non-target stimuli,with a highest achievable score of 4.9.

Bivariate correlation analyses were conducted in SPSS(Version 23.0;IBM corp.,Armonk,NY,USA)to explore associations among demographic,cognitive,MF,and CRF measures.Furthermore,multilevel modeling analyses were utilized to assess fitness associations with cognitive outcomes while accounting for the clustered nature of the data.R software(nlme package,Version 3.1-141;R Core Team,Vienna,Austria)51was used for multilevel modeling,and the coef ficients representing the strength of association between fitness and outcome variables were standardized to facilitate interpretation.As ana prioriapproach,all models included demographic factors known to in fluence physical f i tness during adolescence(i.e.,age,sex,and socioeconomic status)52,53and were adjusted for school-level clustering.This strategy allowed for a robust assessment of the association of each fitness component to cognitive outcomes while accounting for individual differences and possible in fluences of the participants’schools.MF and CRF were individually assessed in the f i rst model(henceforth referred to as Model 1).If the fitness variable significantly predicted a cognitive outcome,a follow-up model was constructed to include the other fitness variable in order to adjust for its potential in fluence on the fitness variable being assessed(henceforth referred to as Model 2).

3.Results

A total of 541 participants were included in the analysis after excluding outliers(i.e.,±3 SD)in overall response accuracy and RT,as well asd’for the 1-back and 2-back tasks.Pearson product-moment correlation analyses were conducted for demographic variables and cognitive outcomes.CRF was positively correlated with congruent flanker response accuracy,non-target 1-back accuracy,and non-target and target 2-back accuracy andd’,as well as negatively correlated with congruent and incongruent flanker RT,and with 1-back nontarget RT.MF showed nearly the same pattern of correlations,with an additional significant negative association with 1-back target RT.

3.1.Muscular fitness

A summary of the multilevel modeling analyses can be found in Table 2.

Table 2 Multilevel modeling summary for MF and cognitive outcomes.

3.1.1.Model 1

MF significantly predicted congruent and incongruent flanker response accuracy as well as congruent flanker RT(allp≤0.05).However,MF did not predict incongruent flanker RT,or interference outcomes for either response accuracy or RT(allp≥0.13).For the 1-back task,MF significantly predicted non-target response accuracy as well as non-target and target RT(allp=0.02).MF also showed a trend ford’(p=0.06).However,MF did not predict target response accuracy(p=0.25).For the 2-back task,MF predicted non-target and target response accuracy as well asd’(allp≤ 0.04).However,MF didnotpredictnon-targetortargetRT(allp≥0.25).

3.1.2.Model 2

Follow-up analyses were conducted on outcomes significantly predicted by MF in Model 1.In these analyses,the association of MF with flanker and n-back outcomes failed to reach signi ficance when CRF was accounted for in the model(all β(standard error(SE))≤0.09(0.05),allp≥0.09),suggesting that the variance accounted for by MF was not independent of CRF.

3.2.Cardiorespiratory fitness

A summary of the full set of multilevel modeling analyses can be found in Table 3.Fig.1 depicts the standardized linear coef ficients with con fidence intervals for the association of CRF with each cognitive outcome observed in Model 2.

3.2.1.Model 1

CRF significantly predicted congruent and incongruent accuracy as well as congruent RT(allp≤0.05)and showed a trend for accuracy interference(p=0.07).CRF did not predict incongruent f l anker RT or RT interference(allp≥0.10).For the 1-back task,CRF significantly predicted all accuracy and RT outcomes across conditions(allp≤0.03).For the 2-back task,CRF predicted nontargetand target accuracy as well asd’(allp<0.001),andshowed a trend for target RT(p=0.09).CRF did not predict non-target RT(p=0.40).

Table 3 Multilevel modeling summary for CRF and cognitive outcomes.

3.2.2.Model 2

Follow-up analyses were conducted on outcomes that CRF significantly predicted in Model 1.CRF remained a significant predictor of incongruent response accuracy performance even when adjusting for MF(β(SE)=0.16(0.05),p=0.002),suggesting that CRF was related to greater incongruent trial response accuracy.However,the association of CRF with congruent trial response accuracy(β(SE)=0.11(0.06),p=0.06)and congruent trial RT(β(SE)=-0.11(0.06),p=0.08)were reduced to a trend when MF was included in Model 2.For the 1-back task,CRF remained a significant predictor of target response accuracy andd’even when adjusting for MF(β (SE)≥ 0.13(0.06),allp<0.03),suggesting that greater CRF was related to better performance on this task condition.The association of CRF with non-target trial response accuracy(β (SE)=0.10(0.06),p=0.09)and RT(β (SE)=-0.12(0.06),p=0.053)was reduced to trends when MF was included in Model 2.The association of CRF with target RT failed to reach signi ficance when adjusting for MF.For the 2-back task,CRF remained a significant predictor of target and non-target response accuracy andd’even when adjusted for MF(all β(SE)≥0.13(0.06),allp<0.04,suggesting that greater CRF was related to better performance across measures of response accuracy on the 2-back task.

4.Discussion

The current study investigated the relationship of older adolescents’physical fitness with their performance on measures of cognitive control.Greater CRF was related to greater response accuracy across tasks that modulated inhibitory control and working memory demands.Speci fically,CRF predicted response accuracy on the incongruent condition of the flanker task,even when MF was accounted for in the model,suggesting that higher levels of CRF are related to greater response accuracy during task conditions that require greater amounts of inhibitory control.Such a finding has been reported previously in preadolescent children.20Greater CRF also predicted working memory performance in the 1-back and 2-back tasks.However,MF did not predict performance on any of the cognitive outcomes when CRF was included in the model.Accordingly,greater levels of CRF,independent of MF,were associated with task performance across tasks that manipulated different aspects of cognitive control.Together,these findings indicate that higher levels of CRF selectively support older adolescents’cognitive operations during engagement in inhibitory control and working memory tasks.

Fig 1.Multilevel modeling CRF associations with cognitive outcomes.Standardized regression coef ficients of CRF in Model 2(adjusted for demographics and MF).*p<0.05.Positivevalues for accuracy,interference,and d’=better performance;negative values for RT=better performance.CRF=cardiorespiratory fitness;RT=reaction time.

Studies that have investigated the association of physical fitness with cognitive control have typically focused on CRF(see reference24for an exception).While the current results support previous findings indicating a positive relationship between CRF and cognitive control,20,22,54,55it was unexpected that MF would not demonstrate an independent relationship with any of the cognitive outcomes.This result contradicts a recent study by Kao and colleagues,24which found a selective association between MF and working memory performance independent of CRF.However,there were notable differences between studies;for example,Kao et al.24assessed preadolescent children and the current study assessed older adolescents.Furthermore,MF was assessed differently across studies,with Kao et al.24using a full-body battery of assessments consisting of 7 different muscle endurance exercises,whereas the current study utilized push-up(muscular endurance)and standing long jump(maximal strength/power)tests.Finally,Kao et al.24used a more valid and precise measure of CRF(i.e.,VO2maxtest),whereas the current study used a field-based measure(i.e.,PACER test)that was more suited for assessing large groups of participants.Despite this limitation,the PACER test remains a valid and reliable assessment of CRF.38Therefore,the current findings may differ from prior reports because of the population sampled or the manner in which MF was assessed.Regardless,research investigating links between MF and cognitive control remains sparse.The hypothesis that MF would bene fit cognition is logical given its link to a multitude of health bene fits,including decreased central adiposity,reduction of metabolic risk factors,improved bone health,molecular growth factors,and psychological factors such as positive self-esteem.56Such health bene fits may support greater cognitive functioning;57,58thus,the notion that MF could bene fit cognitive control remains plausible.Indeed,MF may in fluence aspects of cognition(e.g.,cognitive flexibility)that were not assessed in the current study protocol.Further research is necessary to fully unpack the findings,including the possibility of divergence in the association between MF and cognitive performance across adolescent development.Given that this has only been explored in crosssectional studies,future research should explore causality with more robust study designs.

The literature has been more straightforward regarding the bene ficial relationship between CRF and cognitive control.In preadolescent children,it has been previously established that greater CRF is associated with greater task performance(as measured via response accuracy)on tasks of cognitive control,including inhibition and working memory.20,34,59Furthermore,increased CRF in preadolescent children has most often been reported relative to response accuracy compared to RT for tasks of inhibitory control20and working memory.60,61It is notable that such a pattern of results differs in older adulthood,where RT appears more sensitive during this period of the lifespan.62The current study corroborated previous research in children, finding that greater CRF predicted increased response accuracy across cognitive tasks,particularly during conditions requiring greater amounts of inhibitory control and working memory.However,these findings differ from recent cross-sectional studies of CRF and cognitive control in younger adolescents.54,55For instance,Huang and colleagues55found that higher CRF was associated with shorter RT,rather than greater response accuracy,across congruency conditions in a modi fied f l anker task,whereas Westfall et al.54found that higher CRF was related to greater accuracy and shorter RT across congruency conditions on the same flanker task.Thus,while greater levels of CRF are speci fically bene ficial to response accuracy in preadolescent children,greater CRF in older adolescents may impart varying,more general bene fits to response accuracy and RT.Regardless,the extant literature is consonant in reporting that CRF has a positive relationship with inhibitory control,which is consistent across studies sampling from populations at different stages of development.The current study also provided unique evidence of an association of greater CRF with response accuracy on tasks of working memory in olderadolescents.Furtherresearch may help elucidate whether,similar to inhibitory control,the behavioral bene fits of greater CRF on working memory in adolescents are variable or more speci fic to response accuracy,as observed during preadolescence.Nevertheless,the accumulating work in this area has further implications for adolescents,a population characterized by decreased physical activity behaviors.The current f i ndings support the idea that physical activity is a vital component of the school day,especially given the heightened academic and social pressures that older adolescents often experience.Engaging in activities that support CRF may help to improve cognitive processes that underlie adolescents’scholastic performance and psychological well-being.

As evidence mounts regarding the importance of fitness for cognition,there appears to be a clear advantage in maintaining activities that support CRF throughout childhood and adolescence.The current study supports this notion,albeit with cross-sectional evidence.The effects found in the current study were small yet significant in magnitude.However,even small effects are meaningful when considering that one’s capacity for cognitive control is determined by a vast multitude of internal and external factors.The fact that small positive effects of fitness on cognition have been shown to be consistent and repeatable is inherently meaningful.63Moreover,the current study parceled out other factors in fluential to cognitive performance(i.e.,demographics)in its analyses.Thus,the current findings provide a robust platform for future research on fitness and cognition in older adolescents.A natural progression,therefore,would be to investigate the effect of physical activity interventions on cognitive and academic outcomes.Previous research on long-term,CRF training-centered interventions in preadolescent children has shown a positive effect on cognitive and brain function.34,59Conducting research of this nature provides vital support for prioritizing physical activity in school curricula,especially for older adolescents,whose attendance in physical education classes remains below recommended levels.64Such a trend is counterintuitive to the mounting findings of an association of physical fitness to academic achievement in children and adolescents.14,16,23Moreover,physical activity interventions have also been found to be effective in improving timeon-task behavior in the classroom.65,66Future research should continue to assess how the implementation of physical activity interventions(including physical education),especially those that promote CRF,may support scholastic performance and the underlying cognitive and brain processes.

One of the strengths of the current study is its sample size,albeit with the caveat that data collection occurred within classrooms across different schools.There was potential for selection bias because more of the classes that participated were senior physical education classes rather than other types of classes,which could have resulted in the inclusion of more physically active participants than would be expected in the broader population.There were also limitations imposed by environmental factors.Given the restricted quantity of equipment(i.e.,laptops)and space constraints,cognitive assessments occurred while other participants were in the same room,which may have led to interference from a “busy”testing environment.Experimenters attempted to minimize distractions and supervise participants as much as possible to ensure that they understood and were engaged in the tasks;however,the participants’performance could have been in fluenced by the presence of other people in the room.Collecting data in schools always poses these types of challenges,as well as requiring coordination with students’course schedules.Testing conditions such as those utilized in the current study cannot replicate the control that a lab setting affords.Future research in school settings should endeavor to minimize participant distraction within the context of testing.

5.Conclusion

The current study investigated the associations of older adolescents’physical fitness(i.e.,CRF and MF)and cognitive control in a large sample of older adolescents.The main findings revealed significant associations of CRF on inhibitory control and working memory outcomes,even when MF was accounted for in the model.Greater CRF was associated with greater response accuracy across cognitive tasks,which is consistent with previous findings of a positive relationship between levels of CRF and behavior on tasks of cognitive control.On the other hand,MF did not predict performance on any of the cognitive measures,especially when CRF was accounted for in the model.These results provide support for emphasizing older adolescents’engagement in activities that support CRF as a means for improving their cognitive function.

Acknowledgments

Support for this project was provided by the National Health and Medical Research Council(APP1120518).The study sponsors did not play a role in designing the study;collecting,analyzing,and interpreting the data;writing the report;or deciding to submit the manuscript for publication.

Authors’contributions

All authors participated in the design of the study and contributed to the manuscript writing.AAL,JJS,NE,and DRL contributed to and/or oversaw data collection;TTS,AAL,JJS,and CHH contributed to data reduction/analysis.All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript,and agree with the order of presentation of the authors.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.