Ruo-Chu Xiong, Xin Fu, Li-Zhen Wu, Cheng-Han Zhang, Hong-Xiang Wu, Yu Shi, Wen Wu,
1 Clinical Medicine, First Clinical Medicine School, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou, Guangdong Province, China
2 Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Zhujiang Hospital, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou, Guangdong Province, China
Abstract OBJECTIVE: The objective of this study is to summarize and analyze the brain signal patterns of empathy for pain caused by facial expressions of pain utilizing activation likelihood estimation, a meta-analysis method.DATA SOURCES: Studies concerning the brain mechanism were searched from the Science Citation Index, Science Direct, PubMed, DeepDyve, Cochrane Library, SinoMed, Wanfang, VIP, China National Knowledge Infrastructure, and other databases, such as SpringerLink, AMA, Science Online, Wiley Online,were collected. A time limitation of up to 13 December 2016 was applied to this study.DATA SELECTION: Studies presenting with all of the following criteria were considered for study inclusion: Use of functional magnetic resonance imaging, neutral and pained facial expression stimuli,involvement of adult healthy human participants over 18 years of age, whose empathy ability showed no difference from the healthy adult, a painless basic state, results presented in Talairach or Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates, multiple studies by the same team as long as they used different raw data.OUTCOME MEASURES: Activation likelihood estimation was used to calculate the combined main activated brain regions under the stimulation of pained facial expression.RESULTS: Eight studies were included, containing 178 subjects. Meta-analysis results suggested that the anterior cingulate cortex (BA32), anterior central gyrus (BA44), fusiform gyrus, and insula (BA13) were activated positively as major brain areas under the stimulation of pained facial expression.CONCLUSION: Our study shows that pained facial expression alone, without viewing of painful stimuli,activated brain regions related to pain empathy, further contributing to revealing the brain's mechanisms of pain empathy.
Key Words: nerve regeneration; facial expression; pain empathy; functional magnetic resonance imaging;GringleALE; activation likelihood estimation; brain function imaging; anterior cingulate cortex; anterior central gyrus; fusiform gyrus; insula; neural regeneration
“Empathy” refers to the feeling and understanding of others'emotions and thoughts. Pain, a relatively subjective experience, can elicit our empathy. Empathy for pain is a process in which people perceive, evaluate, and respond to the pain of others (Decety et al., 2008; Loggia et al., 2008).
Study of the brain's mechanisms of pain is progressing because of the development of neurophysiology and the wider application of imaging technology (Davis et al., 1997)in recent years. Empathy induced by pained expressions has realistic significance. Healthcare workers can be alert to patients' pain by watching their facial expressions. Picture-based paradigms (Simon et al., 2008) and cue-based paradigms (Lamm et al., 2011) are the main patterns applied to the study of empathy for pain.
Presenting a series of images evokes empathy for pain from subjects in the former paradigm. Most of the images depict painful injuries in limbs or neutral situations (Kam et al., 2014), and some record pained expressions (Vachon-Presseau et al., 2012). Abstract visuals are applied in the cue-based paradigm. Most studies suggest that the insular and cingulate cortices are activated by empathy (Singer and Lamm, 2009). Brain mechanisms differ because of diverse factors, including positive and negative experience (Enzi et al., 2016). The brain regions activated can change according to changes in activation intensity. However, there are few studies concerning the mechanisms of empathy for pain elicited by pained expressions.
Our aim was to perform a meta-analysis to integrate and interpret the relevant functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies based on the differences in brain patterns elicited by pain. Using the activation likelihood estimation(ALE) method, the activated brain regions of merged samples were analyzed to explore the brain signal patterns associated with empathy for pain.
ALE is a kind of analysis method that is based on the voxel coordinates of the function of the brain regions positioning (Turkeltaub et al., 2002). ALE uses the three-dimension-al Gaussian function smoothness and arranges inspection into the activated regional center coordinates described in multiple papers to define accurate positions of the activated brain regions.
Compared with traditional chart review, the greatest advantage of ALE lies in its objective and quantitative analysis; in other words, ALE works in a voxel-by-voxel fashion(Laird et al., 2009), to avoid the subjectivity of traditional chart review.
Studies written in English were retrieved from Science Citation Index, Science Direct, PubMed, DeepDyve, Cochrane Library, SinoMed, Wanfang, VIP, China National Knowledge Infrastructure, and other databases, such as Springer-Link, American Medical Association, Science Online, and Wiley Online. The research reported herein was undertaken with a time limitation of up to 13 December 2016.
The key words were “facial AND expression AND pain AND empathy AND fMRI (or functional MRI)”. All authors participated in the literature search by computer and secondary search by hand. Unpublished literature was not included.
Inclusion criteria
The studies were randomized controlled trials; the blinding method and the allocation concealment scheme were not taken into account when developing inclusion and exclusion criteria. Studies presenting with all of the following criteria were considered for study inclusion:
(1) Use of fMRI;
(2) The subjects were shown neutral and pained facial expression stimuli;
(3) The experiments involved adult healthy human participants. The basic state was painless.
(4) The subjects were over 18 years of age.
(5) The results were presented in Talairach or Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates.
(6) Multiple studies by the same team as long as they used different raw data.
Exclusion criteria
Studies with one or more of the following conditions were excluded from this analysis:
(1) Studies with data other than only facial expressions or short video stimulation.
(2) In addition to facial expressions, other stimuli were displayed in the picture or video.
(3) Animal studies.
(4) Abstracts, summaries, case reports, minutes of meetings,lectures, and reviews.
(5) Unpublished literature.
The experimental group looked at a picture or short video,showing a volunteer with a pained expression. The control group looked at a picture or short video showing a volunteer with a neutral expression.
The data collected included:
· Title, author, and publication date.
· Experimental group and (if used) control group information: Number of trials, proportion of male and female participants, average age, basic state, interventions, outcome indicators, and scanning methods.
· Brain-related data: Brain area, hemisphere, voxel value (x, y,z), coordinate system, t value, and z value.
· Statistical information: statistical methods, positive and negative activation.
Coordinate transformation
The Lancaster (icbm2tal) transformation in GingerALE 2.3.6 software (Research Imaging Institute, San Antonio, TX,USA) was used to convert all coordinates to the Talairach coordinate system, and then all the converted coordinates were input into GingerALE 2.3.6 software for processing. A single data analysis was performed.
Parameter setting
According to the maximum activation coordinates peak and the three-dimensional Gaussian distribution model, the specific parameters were set as follows: P value ≤ 0.01, minimum cluster volume > 200 mm3. There was sufficient data to calculate brain ALE distribution.
Image processing
Mango 4.0.2 software (Research Imaging Institute) was used to map the three-dimensional coordinate data to the brain anatomical background of the Talairach coordinate space.
A total of 440 studies were retrieved. Based on the title and abstract, 392 studies were excluded, including abstracts, summaries, minutes of meetings, lectures, and reviews, as well as non-fMRI studies. The full text of the remaining 48 studies was read by the two authors. Applying the inclusion criteria,exclusion criteria, and interventions, 36 studies were initially considered and excluded. The full texts of the remaining 12 studies were read for the second time, and 4 studies with repetitive data or of low quality were excluded, leaving eight studies. Eight studies were considered in the final analysis(Budell et al., 1932; Botvinick et al., 2005; Simon et al., 2007;Danziger et al., 2009; Budell and Jackson, 2010; Eickhoff et al.,2012; Sheng et al., 2013; Hadjikhani et al., 2014) (Figure 1).
Figure 1 Flow diagram of literature screening.
Baseline data and quality evaluation are listed in Table 1.
Increased activation in the brain areas under the stimulation of facial expressions of pain (Table 2 and Figure 2) showed 20 foci in eight trials with healthy subjects, including left precentral gyrus (BA44), left inferior frontal gyrus (BA45),bilateral middle temporal gyrus (BA37), right fusiform gyrus (BA37), left thalamus ventral lateral nucleus, bilateral putamen, bilateral amygdala, left cingulate gyrus (BA32),right middle temporal gyrus (BA39), right superior temporal gyrus (BA22), left inferior occipital gyrus (BA18), left middle occipital gyrus (BA18), right caudate body, left inferior occipital gyrus (BA19), right insula (BA13), left middle temporal gyrus (BA21), right middle frontal gyrus (BA46),and left inferior frontal gyrus (BA47). Because the activation in the brain areas of facial expression of pain is affected by age (Martin et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2006), the results are based on subjects in the young and middle ages ranging from 15-47 years.
Pain is a physiological and psychological activity, including the feeling and response to pain (Li et al., 2018). Empathy refers to the process of experiencing the situation of others,so as to feel and understand the emotions of others. Empathy with pain is the individual's perception, judgment, and emotional response to the pain of others (Danziger, 2006).Existing studies using picture-based paradigms are inconsistent. We aimed to explore the sympathetic brain regions based on facial expressions from eight eligible studies. In this study, data from eight eligible studies were analyzed using GingerALE 2.3.6, and images were processed with Mango 4.0.1. Our results showed that the fusiform gyrus, cingulate gyrus, precentral gyrus, ventral lateral nucleus, inferior occipital gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and insula were activated by the sight of a person with a pained expression.
Previous studies have shown that the anterior cingulate gyrus and the insula belong to the painful sympathetic core networks (Gu and Han, 2007), which are responsible for the processing of emotional components of the pain experience (Price, 2000). The cingulate gyrus is associated with somatosensory awareness (especially pain perception related to emotional judgment) (Hsieh et al., 1996; Killgore and Yurgelun-Todd, 2007) and the regulation of physical and visceral activities; the anterior insula is associated with emotion, coordinating BA14, BA15, and BA16 to deal with sensory experience and to identify facial expression (Gornotempini et al., 2001). In this study, the cingulate gyrus(BA32) and inferior insula (BA13) were highly activated by the sight of pained expressions, indicating that, as with other experimental paradigms, the painful sympathetic core networks were also activated when watching others' pained facial expressions. This suggests that simply watching facial expression can activate the pain sympathetic core networks.In addition, the precentral gyrus (BA44) was also highly activated, which may be due to its mirrored neurons (Leube et al., 2001; Lotze et al., 2006). Through this neuronal activity,the observer consciously shows similar expressions or similar brain activities (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Keysers,2009). Therefore, some scholars believe that it is because of the mirror neurons that humans have the ability to empathize (Cheng et al., 2008).
By directly stimulating the brain, the activated brain areas generally include the primary sensory cortex (S1), secondary sensory cortex (S2), insula, and anterior cingulate cortex(Apkarian et al., 2005). The lateral thalamus is also involved in sensory formation (Ro et al., 2007). This study found that the brain areas activated by an expression of pain also include the insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and ventral lateral nucleus. A large number of studies have shown that activation by pain is mainly in the anterior cingulate cortex and insula (Jackson et al., 2005, 2006; Ogino et al., 2006;Moriguchi et al., 2007; Morrison and Downing, 2007; Lamm et al., 2011), and these areas overlap with the responses of the brain to a pained expression. We can conclude that body pain and empathy for pain share some common parts of the nerve pathways, which mainly comprise the emotion-related brain areas.
Table 1 Basic inclusion criteria for articles
Table 2 Brain areas activated by the stimulation of facial expressions of pain
Figure 2 Main activated brain areas for empathy induced by images of pained facial expressions.
The sensory components of the pain experience are processed by the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex(S1, S2), which is based on their anatomical structures and their connection with the subcortical region and the spine; thus, they primarily process and identify sensory stimulation of the body (Rainville et al., 1997; Rainville,2002; Fusar-Poli et al., 2009). Because this study does not involve direct physical pain stimulation, the results do not contain S1 and S2. This reflects that empathy is an emotional experience rather than a somatic sensation. Studies have shown that the secondary sensory cortex and sensory motor cortex are activated only when there is direct stimulation by pain (Singer et al., 2004). These findings also support the findings of this study. However, some studies indicate that even in the absence of actual pain stimuli, the sensorimotor cortex will be activated (Ogino et al., 2006;Gu and Han, 2007). This may result from the differences in research paradigms and stimulus patterns (Lamm et al.,2011). It is noteworthy that some scholars have found that the empathetic brain areas and the emotional components of pain are not exactly the same (Jackson et al., 2006). This may indicate that pain empathy is not equivalent to pain emotional processing.
The brain regions involved in facial recognition may include the fusiform gyrus, inferior occipital gyrus, anterior central gyrus, and insula (Saarela et al., 2007; Zhen et al., 2013). In this study, the brain area associated with simple face recognition is mainly the fusiform gyrus. The fusiform gyrus is located between the temporal gyrus and the hippocampus,with the ability to recognize a face and body. The region within that responsible for face recognition is called the fusiform face area (Kanwisher et al., 1997; Slotnick and White,2013; Axelrod and Yovel, 2015). The fusiform face area can produce similar recognition for non-face visual images (Mc-Carthy et al., 1997; Rossion et al., 2000), which is a prerequisite for facial perception and is associated with the posterior temporal gyrus (BA21), both belonging to the visual cortex.However, the fusiform gyrus is not only activated under the stimulation of facial expressions; although the fusiform face area is a part of the fusiform gyrus and is responsible for face recognition, the fusiform gyrus has further functions,such as processing information on non-facial stimulation.Studies have shown that the fusiform gyrus is also activated in subjects who are watching others' hands and feet (Eidelman-Rothman et al., 2016), indicating that it is widely involved in general recognition.
Investigating studies using the picture paradigm, this analysis is bound to find data involving the activation of visual pathways (Deyo et al., 2004).
The suboccipital region (BA18) is the secondary visual cortex, and together with BA19 constitutes the visual cortex.However, the visual cortex was not highly activated. This indicates that during pain empathy caused by watching a pained expression, the production of visual information is only a relatively minor stage. Combined with the fusiform gyrus, which is highly activated, it can be seen that the recognition and processing of faces and facial expressions are the leading steps: in other words, the activation of the brain area involved in facial recognition distinguishes stimulation by facial expression from stimulation by nonfacial images.
From the results of our study, the occipital lobe, the frontal lobe, the temporal lobe, and the limbic system were differently and strongly activated. Based on the existing studies and combined with this meta-analysis, we can make assumptions that an image of a pained facial expression produces a pain sympathetic activation of the brain regions as follows:
The pained facial expression image can stimulate the emotional network, which produces spontaneous emotion processing. The image activates the occipital lobe first, followed by the temporal lobe, the insula, the limbic system, and other emotional networks. Among them, the frontal lobe processes the emotional stimulation. In other words, the frontal lobe processes awareness through the integration of physical and subjective feelings and objective information (Osborn and Derbyshire, 2010).
In addition, the human brain will produce direct emotions compared with the self's existing emotional experience, and will then recognize the emotions caused directly by some certain stimuli and find the part that belongs to the empathetic feelings. The prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and other brain areas related to memory and cognition were activated after the emotional network was activated. The cingulate gyrus, hippocampal gyrus, and other parts of the pain sympathetic core network are strongly activated, and then the empathy for pain occurs (Zhou et al., 2004).
The limitations of this study are as follows: (1) Despite repeated searches and manual review, the number of included studies is still insufficient: the sample capacity is small, and can easily cause statistical error. (2) Parts of the studies did not test the subjects' ability for empathy (Mazza et al., 2015),leading to a decline in the quality of this article. (3) Because of an insufficient number of articles, we did not divide the stimulating methods into two groups (static pictures and dynamic short videos), and we did not distinguish between a first-person perspective (imagining that you are suffering the same painful experience as a character in the picture)and a third-person perspective (imagine yourself witnessing the suffering of the characters in the picture)..
In conclusion, the ALE analysis showed that the anterior cingulate cortex, insula, anterior central gyrus (BA44), and fusiform gyrus were activated positively in response to images of pained facial expressions. Pained facial expressions activated the brain regions related to pain empathy, including areas of the emotional circuit and facial recognition,further contributing to revealing the brain's mechanisms of pain empathy.
Author contributions:Writing of the discussion section: RCX and LZW; data analysis: XF; data integration: CHZ; image preparation:HXW. All authors approved the final version of the paper.
Conflicts of interest:The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Financial support:This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China, No. 81473769 (to WW), 81772430; a grant from the Training Program of Innovation and Entrepreneurship for Undergraduates of Southern Medical University of Guangdong Province of China in 2016, No. 201612121057 (to WW). The conception, design, execution, and analysis of this manuscript, as well as the preparation of and decision to publish this manuscript, were made independent of any funding organization.
Reporting statement:This study followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement.
Copyright license agreement:The Copyright License Agreement has been signed by all authors before publication.
Data sharing statement:Datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Plagiarism check:Checked twice by iThenticate.
Peer review:Externally peer reviewed.
Open access statement:This is an open access journal, and articles are distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 License, which allows others to remix, tweak, and build upon the work non-commercially, as long as appropriate credit is given and the new creations are licensed under the identical terms.
Open peer reviewer:Chaur-Jong Hu, Taipei Medical University, China.
Additional file:Open peer review report 1.